TEXT C There is a great concern
in Europe and North America about declining standards of literacy in schools. In
Britain, the fact that 30 percent of 16 year olds have a reading age of 14 or
less has helped to prompt massive educational changes. The development of
literacy has far-reaching effects on general intellectual development and thus
anything that impedes the development of literacy is a serious matter for us
all. So the hunt is on for the cause of the decline in literacy. The search so
far has forced on socioeconomic factors, or the effectiveness of" traditional"
versus" modem" teaching techniques. The fruitless march for the cause of the
increase in illiteracy is a tragic example of the saying" They can’t see the
wood for the trees". When teachers use picture books, they are simply continuing
a long-established tradition that is accepted without question. And for the past
two decades illustrations in reading primers have become increasingly detailed
and obtrusive, while language has become impoverished—sometimes to the point of
extinction. Amazingly, there is virtually no empirical evidence to support the
use of illustrations in teaching reading. On the contrary, a great deal of
empirical evidence shows that pictures interfere in a damaging way with all
aspects of learning to read. Despite this, from North America to the Antipodes,
the first books that many school children receive are totally without text. A
teacher’s main concern is to help young beginner readers to develop not only the
ability to recognize words, but the skills necessary to understand what these
words mean. Even if a child is able to read aloud fluently, he or she may not be
able to understand much of it: this is called" barking at text". The teacher’s
takes of improving comprehension is made harder by influences outside the
classroom. But the adverse effects of such things as television, video games, or
limited language experiences at home, can be offset by experiencing "rich"
language at school. Instead, it is not unusual for a book of 30 or more pages to
have only one sentence full of repetitive phrases. The artwork is often
marvelous, but the pictures make the language redundant, and the children have
no need to imagine anything when they read such books. Looking at a picture
actively prevents children younger than nine from creating a mental image, and
can make it difficult for older children. In order to learn how to comprehend,
they need to practice making their own meaning in response to text. They need to
have their innate powers of imagination trained. As they grow
older, many children turn aside from books without pictures, and it is a
situation made more serious as our culture becomes more visual. It is hard to
wean children off picture books when pictures have played a major part
throughout their formative reading experiences, and when there is competition
for their attention from so many other sources of entertainment. The least
intelligent are most vulnerable, but tests show that even intelligent children
are being affected. The response of educators has been to extend the use of
pictures in books and to simplify the language, even at senior levels. The
Universities of Oxford and Cambridge recently held joint conferences to discuss
the noticeably rapid decline in literacy among their undergraduates. Pictures
are also used to help motivate children to read because they are beautiful and
eye-catching. But motivation to read should be provided by listening to stories
well read, where children imagine in response to the story. Then, as they start
to read, they have this experience to help them understand the language. If we
present pictures to save children the trouble of developing these creative
skills, then I think we are making a great mistake. Academic
journals ranging from educational research, psychology, language learning,
psycholinguistic, and so on cite experiments, which demonstrate how detrimental
picture are for beginner readers. Here is a brief selection: The research
results of the Canadian educationalist Dale Willows were clear and consistent
pictures affected speed and accuracy and the closer the pictures were to the
words, the slower and more inaccurate the child’s reading became. She claims
that when children come to a word they already know, then the pictures are
unnecessary and distracting. If they do not know a word and look to the
pictures, which are not closely related to the meaning of the word, they are
trying to understand. Jay Samuels, an American psychologist, found that poor
readers given no pictures learnt significantly more words than those learning to
read with books with pictures. He examined the work of other researchers who
reported problems with the use of pictures and who had found that a word without
a picture was superior to a Word plus a picture. When children were given words
and pictures, those who seemed to ignore the pictures and pointed at the words
learnt more words than the children who pointed at the pictures, but they still
learnt fewer words than the children who had no illustrated stimuli at
all. The youngest readers will quickly develop good reading skills if they______
A.learn to associate the words in a text with pictures B.are exposed to modem teaching techniques C.are encouraged to ignore pictures in the text D.learn the art of telling stories