Young drivers who use cell phones at the wheel drive like
the elderly — with slower reaction times and an increased risk of accidents — a
new study shows. And what’s more, hands-free phones are no safer than handheld
ones, scientists behind the study say. "If you put a
20-year-old driver behind the wheel with a cell phone, their reaction times are
the same as a 70-year-old driver who is not using a cell phone," said David
Strayer, a University of Utah psychology professor and principal author of the
study. "For five years or so we’ve been interested in what happens when someone
picks up a cell phone and starts to drive," Strayer said. One thing that appears
to happen is that phone-using drivers of all ages have significantly diminished
reaction times. They are slower to hit the brakes and more likely to get into
accidents. Subjects took "freeway drives" in a simulator, using
a hands-free mobile phone for half of the drive. "We’re seeing an 18 to 20
percent slowing of reaction times," Strayer explained. "That means if someone is
talking on a phone, it takes them longer to hit the brakes. They are more likely
to get into an accident, and if they do get into one, it might be more severe,
because they won’t be able to decelerate as much. What you’ve effectively done
is made the reactions of a 20-year-old comparable to those of a
70-year-old." Elderly drivers saw similar declines in reaction
times when they took the wheel with phones. In a bit of a surprise, however,
their reactions did not deteriorate at a greater rate than those of their
younger counterparts. "We see in the lab that older adults tend to have slower
reaction times in general and also sometimes have difficulty multitasking
relative to maybe a 20-year-old," Strayer said. But in the study the 20 older
subjects (average age: 70) suffered no greater impairment than their 20 younger
colleagues (average age: 20). Phone users of all ages also took 17 percent
longer to return to the speed of traffic after braking. Such sluggish driving
can affect the likelihood and severity of rear-end collisions and help to create
gridlock, especially when many drivers display such behavior.
Strayer and his group employed only hands-free phones for testing. Some states,
including New York and New Jersey, have enacted safety legislation that
restricts drivers to hands-free mobile phone use. But many researchers say the
taws aren’t enough — and may be completely useless. "We have research that
concludes that the use of a phone, whether handheld or hands-free, can have the
same negative impact," said Rae Tyson, spokesman for the U.S. National Highway
Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA). In fact, NHTSA studies at the University
of Iowa suggested that in some cases, hands-free devices could pose a greater
risk, because tasks like dialing can be more difficult and take longer. The
University of Utah team’s research delivered similar results. "The distinction
some laws make between hands-free and regular phones doesn’t stand up to
scientific scrutiny," Strayer explained. "We’ve done studies, and other studies
in Sweden and Australia have all come to the same conclusion: that it’s a very
similar signature of impairment." The distracting effects of
cell phones are attributed largely to the conversations themselves, which draw a
driver’s attention away from the road. The effect is dubbed inattention
blindness. "Not to say that dialing isn’t a problem, but you can probably
develop work-arounds where your hands are off the wheel for a limited amount of
time," Strayer said. "Drivers engage in multitasks, like eating a sandwich or
tuning the radio, when they perceive a lull in traffic and think it’s safer.
People are not too bad at judging those lulls if it’s a relatively short
activity." But the context of phone conversation seems too big a distraction for
most motorists. "We used an eye tracker to try to see what they were looking at
while talking on the phone," Strayer said. "The measurements show that they
simply aren’t picking up information that’s right in front of them, whether it’s
as mundane as a street sign or even a person or child on the side of
road." Interestingly subjects in earlier studies displayed no
similar distractions when talking to passengers, or listening to the radio or
books on tape. The NHTSA believes that driver distractions of all types are a
factor in probably 25 to 30 percent of crashes, but more specific data are
unavailable. "Our findings and those of others are that driving performance can
be compromised by using wireless communication devices," Tyson said. "In general
there is certainly the potential for deterioration of driving skills if you are
talking and driving at the same time — but nobody knows precisely what the
impact of cell phones has been." Which of the following is INCORRECT about the subjects of the new Utah
study
A. There were at least 40 subjects who were studied on.
B. Half of the subjects used cell phones while the other half didn’t.
C. The subjects were driving in a simulator instead of in the real
roads.
D. The subjects used only hands-free cell phones during the study.